Expert answer:Read this story and Write a 800 wards essay

Answer & Explanation:drunk and Drunker.doc Write about what this story talk about and your thoughts.drunk_and_drunker.docdrunk_and_drunker.doc
drunk_and_drunker.doc

drunk_and_drunker.doc

drunk_and_drunker.doc

Unformatted Attachment Preview

Drunk and Drunker
JAMES HAMBLIN
“I DON’T KNOW what’s wrong with me!” Having cast your merlot across your boss’s
sweater, you futilely thrust a napkin in her direction. You’re no stranger to a drink.
Why now—at the company picnic—has that second glass gone to your head?
Most of us know, for better or for worse, that drinking on an empty stomach, or while
on prescription medications, can leave us unduly inebriated. Less familiar is a series
of external cues that may determine how much we’re affected by alcohol and other
substances. Shepard Siegel, a professor emeritus of psychology, neuroscience, and
behavior at McMaster University in Ontario, coined the term situational specificity of
tolerance in 1976. The idea is that the context in which we have a beer (or a cocktail,
or methadone) helps determine the effects we feel—probably because our bodies
prepare for the physiological reaction produced by a familiar drug when we think that
reaction is imminent. Siegel describes the process as an example of classical
conditioning (like the Ivan Pavlov experiment in which a bell was rung each time a
dog was fed, with the result being that the dog salivated whenever he heard the bell).
Take the panic a few years back over Four Loko, the “blackout in a can,” whose
combination of malt liquor and caffeine was widely blamed for a spate of
hospitalizations. At the time, Siegel argued that the real danger Four Loko posed to
drinkers was not that it mixed alcohol and caffeine, but that it provided alcohol
outside its usual context—with candy-like flavors, in tall, brightly colored cans that
made it look like soda. A 1997 study by other researchers bears out the point: Subjects
were all given the same amount of alcohol, but some of them consumed that alcohol
in the form of beer, while others were plied with an unfamiliar “blue peppermint
mixture.” The latter group—the group that didn’t know what was coming—did
significantly worse on cognitive and motor tests .
Other seemingly innocuous factors also appear to play a role in intoxication. One
study found that if you mix liquor with soda, a diet version leads to higher
blood-alcohol concentrations than does regular soda (the effect seems to hinge on a
digestive mechanism) [3]. Even the shape of the glass you’re holding may affect how
quickly you drink and thus how drunk you get. Researchers at the University of
Bristol found that you’ll be 60 percent slower to drink a fixed amount of alcohol if it’s
in a straight glass as opposed to a curved one.
Beyond the cues provided by a drink and its vessel, the physical environment in
which a substance is consumed may also be important. See, for example, the study
that found that subjects who were given alcohol in an office setting suffered more
from its “deleterious effects” (meaning motor and cognitive impairment) than people
who drank the same amount in a bar . The study also found that heart rate—which
typically rises with intoxication—increased less when people drank alcohol in a
familiar context than it did when the alcohol was delivered in an unexpected context.
The researchers concluded that the anticipation of alcohol probably triggers a
“compensatory response,” whereby the body somehow prepares for and counteracts
the effects of intoxication. In other words: more classical conditioning, but with
martinis.
Even with a familiar substance in a familiar place, environment can still play a
role—at least if you’re a snail, and your substance of choice is an opiate. Snails given
morphine had less tolerance for the drug in colder temperatures . Another study found
that snails placed in a magnetic field became intoxicated more quickly than snails in a
control group. Which raises another question: How do you test the sobriety of a snail?
Drunk and Drunker
JAMES HAMBLIN
“I DON’T KNOW what’s wrong with me!” Having cast your merlot across your boss’s
sweater, you futilely thrust a napkin in her direction. You’re no stranger to a drink.
Why now—at the company picnic—has that second glass gone to your head?
Most of us know, for better or for worse, that drinking on an empty stomach, or while
on prescription medications, can leave us unduly inebriated. Less familiar is a series
of external cues that may determine how much we’re affected by alcohol and other
substances. Shepard Siegel, a professor emeritus of psychology, neuroscience, and
behavior at McMaster University in Ontario, coined the term situational specificity of
tolerance in 1976. The idea is that the context in which we have a beer (or a cocktail,
or methadone) helps determine the effects we feel—probably because our bodies
prepare for the physiological reaction produced by a familiar drug when we think that
reaction is imminent. Siegel describes the process as an example of classical
conditioning (like the Ivan Pavlov experiment in which a bell was rung each time a
dog was fed, with the result being that the dog salivated whenever he heard the bell).
Take the panic a few years back over Four Loko, the “blackout in a can,” whose
combination of malt liquor and caffeine was widely blamed for a spate of
hospitalizations. At the time, Siegel argued that the real danger Four Loko posed to
drinkers was not that it mixed alcohol and caffeine, but that it provided alcohol
outside its usual context—with candy-like flavors, in tall, brightly colored cans that
made it look like soda. A 1997 study by other researchers bears out the point: Subjects
were all given the same amount of alcohol, but some of them consumed that alcohol
in the form of beer, while others were plied with an unfamiliar “blue peppermint
mixture.” The latter group—the group that didn’t know what was coming—did
significantly worse on cognitive and motor tests .
Other seemingly innocuous factors also appear to play a role in intoxication. One
study found that if you mix liquor with soda, a diet version leads to higher
blood-alcohol concentrations than does regular soda (the effect seems to hinge on a
digestive mechanism) [3]. Even the shape of the glass you’re holding may affect how
quickly you drink and thus how drunk you get. Researchers at the University of
Bristol found that you’ll be 60 percent slower to drink a fixed amount of alcohol if it’s
in a straight glass as opposed to a curved one.
Beyond the cues provided by a drink and its vessel, the physical environment in
which a substance is consumed may also be important. See, for example, the study
that found that subjects who were given alcohol in an office setting suffered more
from its “deleterious effects” (meaning motor and cognitive impairment) than people
who drank the same amount in a bar . The study also found that heart rate—which
typically rises with intoxication—increased less when people drank alcohol in a
familiar context than it did when the alcohol was delivered in an unexpected context.
The researchers concluded that the anticipation of alcohol probably triggers a
“compensatory response,” whereby the body somehow prepares for and counteracts
the effects of intoxication. In other words: more classical conditioning, but with
martinis.
Even with a familiar substance in a familiar place, environment can still play a
role—at least if you’re a snail, and your substance of choice is an opiate. Snails given
morphine had less tolerance for the drug in colder temperatures . Another study found
that snails placed in a magnetic field became intoxicated more quickly than snails in a
control group. Which raises another question: How do you test the sobriety of a snail?
Drunk and Drunker
JAMES HAMBLIN
“I DON’T KNOW what’s wrong with me!” Having cast your merlot across your boss’s
sweater, you futilely thrust a napkin in her direction. You’re no stranger to a drink.
Why now—at the company picnic—has that second glass gone to your head?
Most of us know, for better or for worse, that drinking on an empty stomach, or while
on prescription medications, can leave us unduly inebriated. Less familiar is a series
of external cues that may determine how much we’re affected by alcohol and other
substances. Shepard Siegel, a professor emeritus of psychology, neuroscience, and
behavior at McMaster University in Ontario, coined the term situational specificity of
tolerance in 1976. The idea is that the context in which we have a beer (or a cocktail,
or methadone) helps determine the effects we feel—probably because our bodies
prepare for the physiological reaction produced by a familiar drug when we think that
reaction is imminent. Siegel describes the process as an example of classical
conditioning (like the Ivan Pavlov experiment in which a bell was rung each time a
dog was fed, with the result being that the dog salivated whenever he heard the bell).
Take the panic a few years back over Four Loko, the “blackout in a can,” whose
combination of malt liquor and caffeine was widely blamed for a spate of
hospitalizations. At the time, Siegel argued that the real danger Four Loko posed to
drinkers was not that it mixed alcohol and caffeine, but that it provided alcohol
outside its usual context—with candy-like flavors, in tall, brightly colored cans that
made it look like soda. A 1997 study by other researchers bears out the point: Subjects
were all given the same amount of alcohol, but some of them consumed that alcohol
in the form of beer, while others were plied with an unfamiliar “blue peppermint
mixture.” The latter group—the group that didn’t know what was coming—did
significantly worse on cognitive and motor tests .
Other seemingly innocuous factors also appear to play a role in intoxication. One
study found that if you mix liquor with soda, a diet version leads to higher
blood-alcohol concentrations than does regular soda (the effect seems to hinge on a
digestive mechanism) [3]. Even the shape of the glass you’re holding may affect how
quickly you drink and thus how drunk you get. Researchers at the University of
Bristol found that you’ll be 60 percent slower to drink a fixed amount of alcohol if it’s
in a straight glass as opposed to a curved one.
Beyond the cues provided by a drink and its vessel, the physical environment in
which a substance is consumed may also be important. See, for example, the study
that found that subjects who were given alcohol in an office setting suffered more
from its “deleterious effects” (meaning motor and cognitive impairment) than people
who drank the same amount in a bar . The study also found that heart rate—which
typically rises with intoxication—increased less when people drank alcohol in a
familiar context than it did when the alcohol was delivered in an unexpected context.
The researchers concluded that the anticipation of alcohol probably triggers a
“compensatory response,” whereby the body somehow prepares for and counteracts
the effects of intoxication. In other words: more classical conditioning, but with
martinis.
Even with a familiar substance in a familiar place, environment can still play a
role—at least if you’re a snail, and your substance of choice is an opiate. Snails given
morphine had less tolerance for the drug in colder temperatures . Another study found
that snails placed in a magnetic field became intoxicated more quickly than snails in a
control group. Which raises another question: How do you test the sobriety of a snail?

Purchase answer to see full
attachment

How it works

  1. Paste your instructions in the instructions box. You can also attach an instructions file
  2. Select the writer category, deadline, education level and review the instructions 
  3. Make a payment for the order to be assignment to a writer
  4.  Download the paper after the writer uploads it 

Will the writer plagiarize my essay?

You will get a plagiarism-free paper and you can get an originality report upon request.

Is this service safe?

All the personal information is confidential and we have 100% safe payment methods. We also guarantee good grades

Calculate the price of your order

550 words
We'll send you the first draft for approval by September 11, 2018 at 10:52 AM
Total price:
$26
The price is based on these factors:
Academic level
Number of pages
Urgency
Basic features
  • Free title page and bibliography
  • Unlimited revisions
  • Plagiarism-free guarantee
  • Money-back guarantee
  • 24/7 support
On-demand options
  • Writer’s samples
  • Part-by-part delivery
  • Overnight delivery
  • Copies of used sources
  • Expert Proofreading
Paper format
  • 275 words per page
  • 12 pt Arial/Times New Roman
  • Double line spacing
  • Any citation style (APA, MLA, Chicago/Turabian, Harvard)

Our guarantees

Delivering a high-quality product at a reasonable price is not enough anymore.
That’s why we have developed 5 beneficial guarantees that will make your experience with our service enjoyable, easy, and safe.

Money-back guarantee

You have to be 100% sure of the quality of your product to give a money-back guarantee. This describes us perfectly. Make sure that this guarantee is totally transparent.

Read more

Zero-plagiarism guarantee

Each paper is composed from scratch, according to your instructions. It is then checked by our plagiarism-detection software. There is no gap where plagiarism could squeeze in.

Read more

Free-revision policy

Thanks to our free revisions, there is no way for you to be unsatisfied. We will work on your paper until you are completely happy with the result.

Read more

Privacy policy

Your email is safe, as we store it according to international data protection rules. Your bank details are secure, as we use only reliable payment systems.

Read more

Fair-cooperation guarantee

By sending us your money, you buy the service we provide. Check out our terms and conditions if you prefer business talks to be laid out in official language.

Read more

Order your essay today and save 20% with the discount code ESSAYHELP